This page is merely meant to look up instructions of the PsyToolkit scripts. It assumes you know more or less what you are looking for. If you are new to PsyToolkit, it is easier to start looking at some of the examples here or more detailed examples in the experiment library. |
Other relevant documents:
How to use this experiment-script overview page
This page lists all possible elements of a PsyToolkit script. It will also give a short syntax description. A syntax is a description of how instructions can be used. In the syntax description, the compulsory arguments of an instruction are in square brackets []. The optional arguments in normal brackets (). A | sign indicates different valid values for an argument.
For example, take the example for the instruction "show rectangle". The example below shows that there are 4 compulsory arguments: X,Y,W,H. This is the x-coordinate, the y-coordinate, the width and height. The arguments Red, Green, and Blue are optional (by default, a rectangle is white, but you can give it different colors)
show rectangle [X,Y,W,H](Red,Green,Blue|Colorname) |
The brackets in the syntax description are, of course, not typed in the real script; it is just a way of describing a syntax. Look carefully at the examples given for each instruction to see on how to use instructions. |
structure of scripts
There are different sections in scripts, and each section is separated from others by at least one empty line. |
Everything following a hash mark is a comment only for human’s eyes, the computer will ignore it. |
The main sections which contain multiple lines each are:
-
basic options describes handy non-default PsyToolkit settings such as screen size
-
advanced options describes advanced options beginners can ignore
-
bitmaps/sounds/fonts describes which stimuli to use
-
task can be used multiple times to describe paradigm trials
-
table can be used multiple times to describe experimental conditions
-
block can be used multiple times to run blocks of trials
-
blockorder order of blocks can optionally be set in a different way (advanced users only)
basic options
Whenever you program an experiment, you may want to deviate from the default values. For example, the default screen size of an experiment is 800 by 600 pixels, but you can change this using the option "resolution". The available options are listed below. At the bottom of this section is a complete example.
There are many options, but they are mostly for advanced users. If you are new to PsyToolkit, you at first ignore the options. |
resolution
This sets the resolution of the experiment. This default resolution is 800 by 600.
When specifying the resolution, it is width by height, separated only by a space. See example below. |
options
fullscreen
resolution 1000 800
task MyTask
set $x random -300 300 # random x position
set $y random -300 300 # random y position
set $mysize random 10 100
show rectangle $x $y $mysize $mysize 255 0 0 # show red rectangle
show rectangle -100 -100 $mysize $mysize green # show green rectangle
delay 200
clear -1
delay 200
clear -2
block MyBlock
tasklist
MyTask 200
end
You can instead also write the resolution with an x. For example: |
options
resolution 1000x800
frame
This sets the size of the box of the experiment in an online experiment when not in full screen mode. This is rarely needed, but it is nice to know you can adjust this.
In the example below, you work with a 1000 800 resolution. But without the frame option, this would be scaled down to 800 by 600 in the non-fullscreen mode. If you want that in the non-window mode, the box appears bigger, you can adjust that with frame.
options
fullscreen
resolution 1000 800
frame 1000 800
Frame might not be needed often. When you use it, it will most likely have the same numbers as fullscreen. |
scale
The scale option allows you to scale the screen. This is practical in fullscreen mode or for resolutions bigger than the default 800 by 600 pixel area. PsyToolkit will always keep the correct aspect ratio.
When does this take effect?
-
When you go into fullscreen (it will scale up)
-
When you have a larger than 800 x 600 resolution and are not in full screen mode (it will scale down)
options
fullscreen
scale
resolution 1600 1200
background color
You can specify the background color of the screen. By default it is black, but you can set it here to a different color. You can either specify it using a color name (one of red,green,blue,white,yellow,pink,purple,black,orange) or a RGB triplet (like 255 0 0 for red), or a hexadecimal six letter/number combination (like FF0000 for red).
options
background color white
task MyTask
set $x random -300 300 # random x position
set $y random -300 300 # random y position
set $mysize random 10 100
show rectangle $x $y $mysize $mysize 255 255 0 # show yellow rectangle
delay 100
clear -1
block MyBlock
tasklist
MyTask 200
end
options
background color 255 255 255
options
background color FFFF00
fullscreen
Runs experiment in fullscreen mode.
options
fullscreen
task MyTask
set $x random -300 300 # random x position
set $y random -300 300 # random y position
set $mysize random 10 100
show rectangle $x $y $mysize $mysize 255 255 0 # show yellow rectangle
delay 100
clear -1
block MyBlock
tasklist
MyTask 200
end
mouse
By default, the mouse cursor is not shown, because from a psychological point of view it can be a distracting stimulus. But for some experiments, the mouse is needed as a pointing device. This option makes sure the mouse cursor is visible. The only useful value is "on".
options mouse on
The option only switches the mouse on at the start of the experiment. You can still switch it off later in a task using the mouse hide and mouse show instructions. Click here to read more about those two instructions. |
set
This lets you set a global variable. This can be handy if you want to test an experimental parameter (e.g., intertrial interval) to a specific value throughout the experiment. If you do not use the variable anywhere, this set value will just be ignored.
options
fullscreen
set &my_intertrialinterval 100
task MyTask
set $x random -300 300 # random x position
set $y random -300 300 # random y position
set $mysize random 10 100
show rectangle $x $y $mysize $mysize 255 0 0 # show red rectangle
delay 100
clear -1
delay &my_intertrialinterval
block MyBlock
tasklist
MyTask 200
end
Sometimes it is handy to have set some basic parameters in the options as global variables. That way, you can easily find them back. Good examples are intertrial intervals, maximum allowed response times, stimulus size, etc. |
start messages
In the web-based version, every experiment starts with a red box and the English message Click to start. Also, depending on the time it takes to load the messages, you will see a message wait a second.
You can change these two messages with the following two options:
options
startbutton text My own click-to-start message
loading text My own wait-a-second message
advanced options
var in
When you use PsyToolkit within online PsyToolkit surveys, you can exchange numerical variables between the experiments and a survey.
You can only exchange whole-number numerical variables. That is, numbers such as 12 or 3, but not letters, number-letter combinations, and neither fractional numbers (such as 12.1 or 3.4) |
For example, you can have a survey question asking for someone’s age, and then subsequently use the age as a variable in the experiment. You need to specify the survey label. Below is an example.
l: sleep
t: range
q: how many hours sleep did you have?
- {min=0,max=10} Select number
l: alcohol
t: range
q: how many glasses of alcohol did you drink?
- {min=0,max=20} Select number
l: the_experiment
t: experiment
- my_experiment
l: information
t: info
q: Your score is {$xxx} and {$yyy}.
In the following experiment, participants are asked to respond to a rectangle. Let’s assume that sleepy people or those with alcohol need more time, we want to give them more time, so we make the maximum time they can respond in longer. We create a new variable MyMaxTime based on their alcohol consumption and there hours of sleep. The more they have drunk or the less they have slept, the higher &MyMaxTime will be. This just shows how you can use the variables |
Note that the variables sleep and alcohol are treated as global variables, and need to be preceded by the & sign. |
options
var in sleep alcohol
task myTask
keys space
show rectangle 0 0 100 100
set &MyMaxTime expression 1000 + 100 * &alcohol + 100 * ( 10 - &sleep )
readkey 1 &MyMaxTime
save RT
block
tasklist
myTask 10
end
I used spaces, but you can also write this: |
set &MyMaxTime expression 1000+100*&alcohol+100*(10-&sleep)
var out
The option var out can be used to save a value created in an experiment directly to the survey. If you do this, the saved value will be available in the questionnaire spreadsheet.
options
var out my_score
l: my_feedback
t: info
q: Your score was {$my_score}
origin
Visual stimuli are presented on the screen, and you need to give an X and a Y coordinate to place them. The origin option tells the computer where point X=0,Y=0 (or 0,0) is.
By default, the coordinate point 0,0 is at the center of the screen. Alternatively, you can set the top-left corner of the screen as the origin (which was the default in older versions of PsyToolkit).
options
origin topleft
fontdir,datadir,bitmapdir,sounddir
The options fontdir,datadir,bitmapdir,sounddir let you change the folders where the computer expects the stimuli to be saved. You do not need to specify this if the stimuli files are in the same directory as your scripting code.
This option makes most sense if you work in C or have large experiments. |
loading stimuli
The computer needs to know which stimuli you use. Stimuli are typically images or sounds, and these need to be loaded. You can also load fonts. Below is explained on how to load bitmaps, sounds, and fonts.
Bitmap and sound-file names are not allowed to start with a number, are not allowed to contain spaces or special characters (except underscore). |
bitmaps
The bitmaps command lets you define a number of bitmaps you want to use in your experiment. Bitmaps can be of common formats, png, jpg, bmp, and gif (although animations are not shown, instead use videos).
The bitmaps line has no parameters, and is followed by lines describing the bitmap, each bitmap having a name and (optional) a file description.
Do not put anything is quotes, and don’t use spaces in the descriptions or in the filenames.
If you don’t add a filename, it assume that a filename exists with the same name (of type png, jpg, bmp, or gif). In the example below, PsyToolkit assumes a file house.png (or house.jpg, etc) to exist because no further bitmap info is given. This is actually the recommended way of working (it is definitely easiest).
Alternatively, you can have a different filename.
bitmaps
house
funnyface funny.bmp
cookie stimuli/cookie.jpeg
smiley
task MyTask
show bitmap funnyface
show bitmap cookie 100 0
Note that for Javascripted experiments in a browser, there are two additional ways to load bitmaps:
1: using the http:// prefix (thus, instead of a full filename, give the URL). This makes sense under very special circumstances, and is not generally recommended, unless you really know what you are doing. The reason for this is that this can speed up download times of online experiments considerably. This because per default, all stimuli are embedded in the HTML file via data:uri, which can lead to large HTML files.
Here is how you do it in PsyToolkit
bitmaps
house http://www.my-website.com/house.png
task MyTask
show bitmap house
2: using data:image/ uris
For more information on how data URI works see data URI on Wikipedia
The data uri is only for very rare and special occasions, and is generally not recommended (because it is not really necessary, but there are some unusual technical reasons why someone might want it under super-unusual conditions) |
bitmaps
testimage data:image/png;base64,iVBORw0KGgoAAAANSUhEUgAAAAQAAAADCAIAAAA7ljmRAAAAGElEQVQIW2P4DwcMDAxAfBvMAhEQMYgcACEHG8ELxtbPAAAAAElFTkSuQmCC
task MyTask
show bitmap testimage
move
move and relocate are not often used. It is mostly used for basic animations and then are very practical. |
move does not give a smooth animation, it is just a change of location. See the simple animations lesson for details. |
Sometimes, you want to change the location of a stimulus that you have already created on screen. This can be useful for simple animations.
You can do this with two different functions in tasks:
-
move
-
relocate (described next)
The following example shows a stimulus at screen center and after 1000 ms moves the stimulus 20 pixels to the right, while not changing the y location. Thus, the move gives the X and Y amount of change (whereas relocate gives a complete new X/Y position).
bitmaps
smiley.png
task MyTask
show bitmap smiley
delay 1000
move 1 20 0
relocate
Relocate is very similar to move, except that you just give the new coordinates.
bitmaps
smiley.png
task MyTask
show bitmap smiley
delay 1000
relocate 1 100 150
sounds
This works like the "bitmaps" commands, just to tell the computer which sounds are being loaded. You can use any sound file format, although the mp3 file format is the one that is most widely used and recommended for online studies. MP3 files are much smaller than WAV files (MP3 files are compressed).
sounds
barkingdog bark.wav
phone ringingphone.mp3
task MyTask
sound phone
delay 300
If you use common file formats mp3, wav, ogg, or flac, you actually do not need to specify the file format, but you can if the file starts with the same as the name you use. So you could make the above code simpler as follows (PsyToolkit will itself figure out the file extension. |
sounds
bark
ringingphone
task MyTask
sound phone
delay 300
As for bitmaps, you can also load a sound from a web server (using http://). The advantage is that this leads to quicker loading times. |
You can also use sounds in the block, for example to play some music when the participant is doing the task. This can be used to test the effect of different types of sounds, music, background noise on task performance. See sound in a block. |
set $x duration my_sound
play my_sound
delay $x
Instead of the word sound, you can also write play, it means exactly the same. |
sound frequencies
Apart from playing sound files, you can also play specific tones or frequencies. This also uses the sound instruction, but now followed by the word freq. Below are various examples:
sound freq sine 400 1000
sound freq sine 400 2000
sound freq triangle 700 1500
sound freq sawtooth 600 500
How to stop a frequency sound. For this, you use the silence instrcuction, but you must use a negative number. The last started sound is -1, the one started before that -2, etc. See example below.
sound freq sine 400 5000 delay 500 sound freq sawtooth 700 5000 delay 500 silence -1 # stops the sawtooth delay 500 silence -2 # stops the sine
fonts
By default, without telling the computer what font you want to use, PsyToolkit will use Arial 20 points. You do not need to define a font if you do not want to anything else than Arial of size 20 points. |
Here you can describe which fonts to use. You can use built-in fonts (arial, times, courier) or load ttf fonts (since 2.3.3 also in Javascript). If you use them online, you need to upload your ttf just like you upload any other stimuli.
Note that the same fonts go by different names. If you are not familiar with the most common types, check WikiPedia for information about Arial, information about Times New Roman, and information about Courier. |
You can use one of the three default font types: arial, times, or courier. If you use one of these three fonts, you do not need to upload or include a font file, because these fonts are already available on computers. |
fonts
arial 18
myfont times 20
mysmallfont arial.ttf 20
mybigfont arial.ttf 40
task MyTask
font mysmallfont
show text "hello"
delay 1000
font mybigfont
show text "world"
delay 1000
font arial
show text "Some text in standard arial"
font myfont
show text "Some text in Times New Roman"
delay 1000
See also show text.
videos
You can use videos in your experiment. You need to tell PsyToolkit where the videos are stored on the internet. You cannot store them on the PsyToolkit server itself.
It is easy to upload a video file to Dropbox. There is a lesson on how to do this in detail.
You can give the video any name, for example, in the example below clip1 and clip2 were chosen, but it could just as well be my_video1 and my_video2.
videos
clip1 https://www.dropbox.com/s/kzo7sx7hkdai16i/beetle.mp4?dl=0
clip2 https://www.dropbox.com/s/bzbs6qpk7ht9yyu/frog.mp4?dl=0
tasks
In PsyToolkit, a task (typically) describes the sequence of exactly one trial of an experiment.
This includes showing or playing audio stimuli, waiting for key/mouse presses, time delays, if/then loops and all sorts of other things.
Tasks are the most difficult part of the scripting language, because they involve writing real sequential computer instructions to show stimuli, wait for responses, etc. Lots of things are possible, and there are many different instruction types. Each instruction type is described below.
Before describing the instructions, a number of fundamental elements of the language are described, namely the structure of tasks, variables, and timestamps.
how tasks are structured
Tasks are really used to program what happens in one trial of a experiment paradigm. A task is just a sequence of instructions, ended with an empty line. The instructions contain the following major parts of information:
-
Where a description of the different experimental conditions is. This can be done with the table statement
-
How exactly the participant should respond (e.g., which keyboard keys, or with the mouse). This can be done, for example, with keys and readkey.
-
The stimuli being used, and when and where they should be presented. This can be done with show and sound.
-
Which data should be saved to a file for later data analysis. This is done with the instruction save
Showing stimuli
Showing (or playing audio) stimuli in PsyToolkit is very simple. The following types of stimuli can be shown:
the concept of variables
Like any computer language, you can use variables. There are global and local variables. You can in principle always use global variables. Local variables are only used within one task, whereas global variables can be used anywhere in the task and block code (see also blocks). You can set initial values of global variables in the options.
You can set a variable with the "set" instruction.
task MyTask
set $x 100
set &y 10
show bitmap FunnyFace $x &y
Variables can only have integer values. That is, an integer, which is a whole number. For example, $x is 100, it cannot be 100.342, because the latter is not a whole number. |
The equal sign is not being used to assign values to variables! That is, no "=" symbol like you would do in most programming languages. |
Variables are not allowed to start with a number, are not allowed to contain spaces, minus signs, dots, or special characters except the underscore. |
Good examples.
-
$MyVariable1
-
$my_variable_1
-
$xyz
The following are NOT allowed!!!!!!!
-
$My Variable 1 (reason: no spaces allowed)
-
$My.variable.1 (reason: no dots allowed)
-
$My-variable-1 (reason: no minus signs allowed)
-
$My*variable (reason: no special characters such as "*" allowed)
-
$123my_variable (reason: not allowed to start with a number)
There are also special types of variables written in capitals, such as RT, STATUS, TABLEROW, TASKNAME, BLOCKNAME, BLOCKNUMBER, BLOCKORDER. They just give you access to important information about a response or the task condition. |
Since 3.3.1, you can use text variables, which start with the % character. See example in the set documentation. |
the concept of timestamps
Timestamps are a special type of variable, but they are only used in very advanced scripts and are normally not necessary. Whenever you set a timestamp, the current time is being saved.
The set function
With set, you can assign values to variables.
set [$&][variablename] [new value, variable, or expression] |
As explained about in "variables", the set function can set the value of variables.
A variable is a basic element in any programming language. A variable is simply a symbol which holds a value. For example, x = 10. PsyToolkit can only work with whole numbers as values. |
set is a relatively advanced function, and when learning PsyToolkit, you do not immediately need it, and you can write scripts without set. |
A global variable is one that remains its value throughout your PsyToolkit script. A local variable is one that is set back to zero every time the task starts again. If this is something you think is too difficult to remember, just use global variables. |
set $x 10
set &x 10
In the rest, the local or global nature of the variable is irrelevant for explaining set, and we just work with $x
set $x &y
set $x 10
set $x increase 2 # after this, $x has value 12
set $x 10
set $x decrease # after this, $x has value 9
set $x expression $a * 3 + 4
It is easier to read if you use spaces between the things on an expression line. But in principle, you can do it without spaces. |
set $x expression $a*3+4
set $x random 1 10
set $x random 2 100 2
set $x random from 1 2 3 5 10
set $x time-since-start
timestamp MyTime1
delay 500
timestamp MyTime2
set $x timestamp-diff MyTime1 MyTime2 # now x should be 500
timestamp MyTime1
dely 2500
timestamp MyTime2
set $x timestamp-seconds MyTime2 # value should be 2
set $x timestamp-milliseconds MyTime2 # value should be 2500
set $x unix-time
save $x
There are two examples of how to get the number of the stimulus currently under the bitmap. The first method is recommended. The second method does the same, but takes more code.
In the example below, there are two stimuli on screen. The $x will get the value where the participant clicked (stimulus 1 is yourface and stimulus 2 is smileyface).
bitmaps
myface
yourface
smileyface
task test
show bitmap yourface -100 0 ## first bitmap
show bitmap smileyface 200 200 ## second bitmap
readmouse l 1 1000
set $x UNDER_MOUSE
bitmaps
myface
yourface
smileyface
task test
show bitmap yourface -100 0 ## first bitmap
show bitmap smileyface 200 200 ## second bitmap
readmouse l 1 1000
set $myMouseX MOUSE_X # current mouse x-coordinate
set $myMouseY MOUSE_Y # current mouse y-coordinate
set $x bitmap-under-mouse $myMouseX $myMouseY ## $x should be 1
You can set text variables. Text variables always start with the % character, just to tell PsyToolkit that we have a text variable here.
You can create a new text variable by pasting different pieces together and then show this. This is particularly useful when giving a participant feedback about RT or numbers of errors, as in the example below.
task test
keys x
show rectangle 0 0 100 100 255 0 0
readkey 1 2000
set $myvar random 1 100
set %y "Hello " RT " " $myvar
show text %y
delay 2000
block testx
tasklist
test 2
end
set $x duration my_sound
task test
keys x
show rectangle 0 0 100 100 255 0 0
readkey 1 2000
set $myvar random 1 100
set %y "Hello " RT " " $myvar
set %k %y[1,5]
show text %k
delay 2000
block testx
tasklist
test 2
end
For substrings, you use an indexing system:
set %text1 "PsyToolkit"
set %text2 text1[1] → text2 is "P" set %text2 text1[-1] → text2 is "t" (last letter) set %text2 text1[-2] → text2 is "i" (second last letter)
set %text2 text1[1,-1] → text2 is "PsyToolkit" (first to last letter) set %text2 text1[1,3] → text2 is "Psy" (letter 1 to letter 3) set %text2 text1[-3,-1] → text2 is "kit" (third last to last letter)
set an array variable
These are for somewhat more experienced users. Make sure you first understand the set instructions. |
In PsyToolkit, an array is simply a serie of whole numbers, such as 1 2 3 4 5. |
Each array variable is a global variable, that is, they stay the same throughout trials and different tasks.
In short, you can apply the following functions (more detailed examples are further below):
instruction |
what it does |
example |
values |
set array to sequence of values |
set &&x 4 2 10 -1 |
sort |
sort sequence in array |
set &&x sort |
range |
fill an arrange with a range |
set &&x range 1 5 |
shuffle |
shuffle an array’s values randomly |
set &&x shuffle |
remove |
remove one value from an array |
set &&x remove first |
sample |
take n values from an array randomly |
set &&x &&y sample 10 |
uniq |
remove all values occuring more than once |
set &&x uniq |
append |
add a value to end of array |
set &&x append 10 |
prepend |
add a value to beginning of array |
set &&x prepend 10 |
clear |
remove all values of an array |
set &&xx clear |
value times n |
repeat a value (or array) multiple times |
set &&x 10 times 5 |
All instructions in this subsection apply to whole arrays (text or numeric) only. There is a separate section for how to extract values from arrays. |
Here are some examples for creating arrays or for taking them apart.
task test
set &&my_array 1 2 5 2 4 1 0 10
task test
set &&my_array clear
task test
set &&my_array range 1 5
The instruction below removes a value from the array, but you will not know which one (see below for the other remove to do that). |
task test
set &&my_array 1 2 3 4
set &&my_array remove value 3
task test
set &&my_array 1 2 3 4
set &&my_array remove first
task test
set &&my_array 1 2 3 4
set &&my_array remove last
task test
set &&my_array 10 4 20 100
set &&my_array remove position 2
task test
set &&my_array remove random
task test
set &&my_array 2 times 5
task test
set &&my_array random 5 10 times 3
You can also repeat arrays. Thus, in the following example we have an array &&my_numbers with the content 1 2 3. After doing this times 3, we have 1 2 3 1 2 3 1 2 3.
task test
set &&my_numbers 1 2 3
set &&my_array &&my_numbers times 3
task test
set &&my_numbers sort
task test
set &&my_numbers sort reverse
task test
set &&my_numbers uniq
You can also use text arrays, start them with %%, see example below. |
task test
set %%my_list "Hello" "this" "is" "PsyToolkit"
task test
set %sentence "This is a sentence"
set %%words split %sentence
You can only split using the space separator. |
set a variable to a value from an array
These are for somewhat more experienced users. Make sure you first understand the set instructions. |
In PsyToolkit, an array is simply a serie of whole numbers, such as 1 2 3 4 5. |
code | what it does |
---|---|
locate |
find the location of a value in an array |
remove |
take and remove from array (the first, the last, a random one, or one at a specific array position) |
use |
use but do not remove from array (the first, the last, a random one, or one at a specific array position) |
size |
get the number of items in an array |
join |
set the text variable to the joined elements of text array |
mean |
get mean value of numbers |
roundmean |
get rounded mean value of numbers |
min |
get min value of number array |
max |
get max value of number array |
sum |
get sum of number array |
sd |
get sd value of number array |
In the example below, we can set an area to the values 4 5 2 10. In this array, the second value (i.e., position 2) contains 5. Thus, &x will equal 2 after this.
task test
set &&my_array 4 5 2 10
set &x &&my_array locate 5
In the following example, the global variable &x will get value 4 (the first), and the array &&my_array will be reduced to 5 2 10.
task test
set &&my_array 4 5 2 10
set &x &&my_array remove first
In the following example, the global variable &x will get value 10 (the last), and the array &&my_array will be reduced to 4 5 2.
task test
set &&my_array 4 5 2 10
set &x &&my_array remove last
In the following example, the global variable &x will get value 5 (which is at the removed position 2). The array will then be 4 2 10
task test
set &&my_array 4 5 2 10
set &x &&my_array remove 2
Note, instead of remove you can use the use instruction. The only difference is that the array would not be modified.
Note, you can also remove a value at random.
task test
set &&my_array 7 5 2 10
set &x &&my_array size
task test
set %%my_array "Smith" "hello" "John"
set %%my_array sort
set %my_sentence %%my_array join " "
Summary of set
Summary of all set functions.
Set is not needed for beginners! It really allows flexibility in your code, if you want and need it. |
For each set function, you can use local or global variables. Do not worry about the difference between local and global variables; when in doubt, use global variables (starting with the & sign). |
purpose | example | result |
---|---|---|
basic use |
||
assign value to local variable |
set $x 10 |
$x contains 10 (only whole values are allowed) |
assign value to global variable |
set &g 12 |
&g contains 12 |
assign value from other variable |
set &h RT |
assuming there was a readkey command earlier, set &h to the value of RT |
assign random value |
set &x random 1 5 |
&x contains one value from 1,2,3,4,5 |
assign random value in steps |
set &x random 1 10 2 |
&x contains one value from 1,3,5,7,9 |
increase variable’s value |
set &x increase |
assume &x already contains 5, after this command it will contain 6 |
increase variable’s value by value |
set &x increase 3 |
assume &x already contains 5, after this command it will contain 8 |
decrease |
set &a decrease |
works the same as increase, except it decrease the value |
assign value based on expression |
set $y expression $x * 3 + 5 |
assume $x contains value 5, then $y will contain 20 |
advanced use: time functions |
||
time since start of experiment in ms |
set $j time-since-start |
if your experiment started 5 seconds ago, $j will contain 5000 |
calculate difference between 2 time stamps |
set $t timestamp-diff MyTime1 MyTime2 |
Assume that MyTime1 is set earlier than MyTime2, $t will contain difference in milliseconds. |
calculate difference between 2 time stamps in seconds |
set $t timestamp-seconds MyTime1 |
the timestamp in seconds |
calculate difference between 2 time stamps in milliseconds |
set $ms timestamp-milliseconds MyTime1 |
the timestamp in milli seconds |
advanced use: mouse functions |
||
find number of stimulus clicked |
set $b bitmap-under-mouse |
if in the last readmouse stimulus 5 was clicked, $b will contain 5 |
find number of stimulus clicked, but only consider certain stimulus numbers |
set $b bitmap-under-mouse range 10 15 |
if in the last readmouse stimulus 11 was clicked, $b will not contain 11. Other stimuli will be ignored. This is handy for overlapping stimuli. |
advanced use: other |
||
find number of last shown stimulus |
set $c show-counter |
if you have shown 10 stimuli, $c will contain 10 |
In the expression, you can use a lot of special mathematical functions, here they are all listed:
purpose | example | result |
---|---|---|
basic use |
||
addition |
set $x expression 3 + 4 |
sets $x to value 7 |
subtraction |
set $x expression 5 - 2 |
sets $x to value 5 |
multiplication |
set $x expression 3 * 4 |
sets $x to value 12 |
division |
set $x expression 10 / 3 |
sets $x to value 3 (automatically rounds) |
power |
set $x expression 2 ^ 5 |
sets $x to 32 |
sqrt |
set $x expression sqrt 9 |
sets $x to 3 |
modulo |
set $x expression 10 % 3 |
sets $x to remainder value 1 |
log |
docu forthcoming |
|
log2 |
docu forthcoming |
|
trigonometry |
||
sin |
docu forthcoming |
|
cos |
docu forthcoming |
|
tan |
docu forthcoming |
|
acos |
docu forthcoming |
|
asin |
docu forthcoming |
|
atan |
docu forthcoming |
|
atan2 |
docu forthcoming |
|
special |
||
abs |
docu forthcoming |
|
ceil |
docu forthcoming |
|
floor |
docu forthcoming |
|
min |
docu forthcoming |
|
max |
docu forthcoming |
commonly used instructions
There are many different instructions for showing stimuli, recording and saving responses. Below they are all listed with a short example of their use. This section describes the ones that are most often used, and that you really need to understand.
Before we start, here is an alphabetical list of the most commonly used instructions:
-
presenting stimuli
-
measuring responses and response times
-
keys tells the computer which keys are being used in a task
-
readkey waits for one keyboard press
-
readkeys waits for multiple keyboard presses
-
textbox allows entry of text, including special characters
-
readmouse waits for a mouse click or mouse movement
-
choose fancy multiple stimuli selection with mouse
-
rate easy way to include Likert scale in your experiment
-
-
waiting for some time
-
delay wait period in milliseconds
-
-
saving data
-
save saves data to file
-
-
telling the computer about experimental conditions
-
table specifies where the experiment conditions are
-
-
conditionals
-
if do something depending on variable value(s)
-
while do something depending on variable value(s)
-
set set a variable
-
set_array set an array variable
-
set_from_array set a variable to a value from an array
-
Below, each of these commands is given in more detail.
keys
keys [list of keycodes] |
At the beginning of a task description, you should tell the computer which keys from the keyboard participants might have to press. This line should be one the first lines in the task description.
Keys given will be associated a keyboard numbers, starting with one. You can use this when saving data. The variable KEY will contain this value when a key has been pressed.
keys a z
List of all the keys available on all platforms:
-
lettes: a to z
-
numbers: 0 to 9
-
special keys: enter capslock tab space end home insert
-
special keys: escape slash backslash quote comma period
-
arrow keys: up down right left
-
numerical keyboard (keypad): kp0 kp1 kp2 kp3 kp4 kp5 kp6 kp7 kp8 kp9
-
number keyboard: kp_period kp_slash kp_star kp_minus kp_plus kp_enter
The following keys are not availble in the Javascript mode
-
shift keys: lshift rshift
-
control keys: lcontrol rcontrol
-
alt keys: lalt ralt
-
logo keys: lsuper rsuper
Mouse on or off
You can switch the mouse cursor off (hide) or on (show). Look at the following lines you can use in a task.
task MyTask show bitmap smileyface mouse hide delay 1000 show bitmap smileyface mouse show delay 1000
table
table [name of table] |
The table simply refers to a table instruction (which is separately defined from the task, and it will contain information about the different experimental instructions).
Each line in a table will be considered the description of a different experimental condition; therefore, tables allow you to have one task description for different conditions. This way you can vary the color, position, or size of stimuli. It is a key element in PsyToolkit scripts, although you can write scripts without tables as well!
If you have more than one task and more than one table, you need to specify in your task which table belongs to it. |
The table row chosen on a given trial is in the variable TABLEROW.
table MyTable
tablerow
This is an advanced function. You normally do not need it. |
The tablerow will be randomly chosen on each trial by PsyToolkit (depending on what use ask for in the tasklist lines). You can fully override this by using the instruction tablerow followed by a number specifying which tablerow to use.
It is not recommended to use this instructions unless you really know what you are doing.
This function can go wrong if you set a tablerow that is not existing. For example, if your table has four rows, but you give the instruction tablerow 10, it is not clear what would happen.
show
show [bitmap|text|rectangle|circle|video](X,Y)(…) |
This is a key instruction, which can show stimuli of the following types:
The number of parameters given to a show instruction can vary.
show bitmap
show bitmap [bitmap](X,Y) |
Imagine you have loaded a bitmap in the bitmaps section of your task description. Now you can show it. Just specify the bitmap, and the bitmap will be shown at screen center. You can also specify the X and Y coordinates (optional).
show bitmap MyBitmap
show bitmap MyBitmap 200 10
More information about how to show bitmaps is available in this lesson. |
show rectangle
show rectangle X,Y,Width,Height, Red,Green,Blue |
You can display colored rectangles, specifying the X and Y position as well as the Width and Height. You also need to specify the Red, Green, and Blue values. That means you will have 7 more values!
In the example below, a rectangle is show at position 0,0 and it has both a width and a height of 100 pixels. The red/green/blue values are set to 255/0/0. The maximum value in each of these three color channels is 255. Given that only red is specified, the rectangle will be red.
PsyToolkit uses the Red-Green-Blue (RGB) color model. Each color can be specified as a combination of red, green, and blue. For each of these three basic colors, you need to give a value between zero and 255. You can create any color, see RGB model on wikipedia. There are websites where you can create your own colors: |
Color | Red | Green | Blue |
---|---|---|---|
White |
255 |
255 |
255 |
Pure red |
255 |
0 |
0 |
Pure blue |
0 |
0 |
255 |
Pure green |
0 |
255 |
0 |
Yellow |
255 |
255 |
0 |
Grey |
128 |
128 |
128 |
Orange |
255 |
128 |
0 |
Pink |
255 |
100 |
180 |
show rectangle 0 0 100 100 255 0 0
show circle
Show a circle. Similar to show rectangle.
show circle X,Y,Radius, Red,Green,Blue |
The 3rd parameter is the radius in pixels (thus, the width is twice the radius).
show circle 0 0 100 255 0 0
Instead of R G B color values, you can use a color name
show circle 0 0 100 red
show line
Show a line. For this, you need to specify the X and Y coordinates of the beginning and end point of a line.
You can specify the color (using Red Green Blue notation)
task testline
show line 10 10 200 10 255 255 0
delay 5000
clear -1
task testline
show line 10 10 200 10 30 255 255 0
delay 5000
clear -1
task testline
line color 150 150 150 ## grey
show line 10 10 200 10
delay 5000
clear -1
task testline
line color 0 255 0 ## green
line width 20
show line 10 10 200 10
delay 5000
clear -1
show background
show background (Red,Green,Blue) |
There is also the easier way to set this using the option background color. |
Sometimes you want the whole screen to have a specific color. In principle, you could just draw a rectangle the size of the screen. The "show background" function does exactly that. You just give the three color parameters and the whole screen will be filled with that color. Otherwise, it is treated like a normal screen stimulus and it has a number and it can be cleared.
show background 100 100 100 ## creates a grey background
delay 500
show rectangle 100 200 200 0 0 ## draw red rectangle
delay 1000
clear -1 ## erase the the rectangle
You do not need to clear the background and the background will stay on until next trial (which is most likely what people want) |
Instead of an R G B triplet, you can also use a color name. |
show background blue ## creates a blue background in a task
show text
The show text instruction shows text.
table MyFirstTable
10 "some text"
15 "some other text"
task MyTask
show text "Some green text" 0 0 0 255 0
show text @2 0 0 0 255 0
By default, without telling the computer what font you want to use, PsyToolkit will use Arial 20 points. You do not need to define a font if you do not want to anything else than Arial of size 20 points. |
There are lots of freely available ttf fonts available on the web. For example via Font Squirrel. |
If you want, you can specify a font and font size as well in the fonts section
show text [text](X,Y)(Red,Green,Blue) |
fonts
MyArial comic.ttf 20
table MyFirstTable
10 "some text"
15 "some other text"
task MyTask
font MyArial
table MyFirstTable
show text "Some green text" 0 0 0 255 0
show text @2 0 0 0 255 0
The default alignment is "center". That means that if you ask something to presented at, say, position 10,10, that is where the center of the word is. See the example below on how to set the alignment. Also, note that you can set the text color for all subsequent "show text" commands, and here you can use a color word. |
While "show text …" is for actually displaying the text, you can set the alignment of color separately. That is easy if you for example want to present multiple texts in the same color.
fonts
arial 20
task MyTask
text color yellow
text align center
show text "Some text with alignment: center" 0 -100
text align left
show text "Some text with alignment: left" 0 0
text align right
show text "Some text with alignment: right" 0 100
delay 10000
block test
tasklist
MyTask 1
end
The following color words are currently implemented: white, yellow, red, green, blue, pink, purple, black, grey, orange, pink. |
For "text color", you can also specify the color in three decimal values or as 6 digit hex code. See example below for the same statement of specifying yellow in three different ways. This example makes no sense in a real experiment (because why would you specify the same color 3x in a row?), but just demonstrates how to use the "text color" statement. |
# specify color as word:
text color yellow
# specify color as Red Green Blue code:
text color 255 255 0
# specify color as Hexadecimal value:
text color FFFF00
Alternatively, you can use a color name directly in the show text line as follows:
show text "Hello" 0 0 red
show video
show video my_clip
show video my_clip -100 100
videocontrol my_clip mute
show video my_clip -100 100
clear
clear [bitmap occurrence](list of bitmap occurrences) |
To erase a stimulus you put on the screen with "show", you can erase with clear. The call "clear" takes at least one parameter. The number corresponds to the count of show calls. For example, if you want to erase a particular bitmap which was shown as the second show event in your task, use "clear 2". Alternatively, you can use negative referral numbers to refer to preceding stimuli: -1 refers to the last presented bitmap! Using the negative referrals is actually much easier to use, because that way you do not need to count bitmaps.
The bitmap counter is reset every trial! |
show bitmap redcircle
show bitmap greencircle
delay 500
clear 2 # clears the second presented bitmap
# or
show bitmap redcircle
show bitmap greencircle
delay 500
clear -1 # clears last presented bitmap, that is "green circle"
# or
show bitmap redcircle
show bitmap greencircle
delay 500
clear 1 2 # clears both bitmaps
Sometimes, you want to clear so many stimuli that it is not nice to have to type the numbers in. If they are in a range, you can specify the range. For example, clear range 1 10 clears stimuli 1 2 3 4 5 6 8 9 10. |
Rarely, you want to clear the whole screen. This is not recommended for time critical things, because it can be slower that changing small parts of the screen. Sometimes you want to clear the whole screen at the beginning or end of trial. You can use clear screen to do so. |
hide/unhide
The hide functions does exactly what the clear function does, except that this way you can also unhide the stimulus again.
Using hide, you do not change the stimulus count of the stimulus. Sometimes, this can be handy.
rotate
It is possible to rotate bitmaps presented with the "show bitmap" command. A good example is given in the Mackworth example in the PsyToolkit experiment library.
Angle is indicated in tenth of degrees. Thus, a 90 degree angle is indicated by 900. |
rotate next 900
show bitmap MyBitmap
show bitmap
delay 500
rotate 1 900
delay
The delay instruction pauses the program for the specified number of milliseconds. You will need this function often, for example, if you want to show a stimulus for a specific time interval, or for waiting between trials.
delay [milliseconds] |
show bitmap MyBitmap
delay 200
clear -1
The parameter of delay (in the example 200) can be a variable or a table entry as well. Look at the examples to learn how to do that. |
readkey
The readkey instruction tells the computer to wait for a keyboard response. The first argument, the correct key, corresponds to the keys in the keys instruction.
readkey [correct key number][maximum RT] |
In the following example, there are two possible keys in the task (named MyTask), the a and the z key. Imagine that a stimulus requires the z button to be pressed, that is the second key (with a being the first in the line "keys"). The readkey command will now way 3 seconds for a key press. If the participants presses the z key, the STATUS will be set to CORRECT (which equals numerical value 1). If the participants presses the wrong key, it will be WRONG (numerical value 2), and if there is no response at all within 3000 ms, the STATUS code will be TIMEOUT (numerical value 3).
task MyTask
keys a z
show bitmap PressTheZkey
readkey 2 3000
After a call to readkey, the user can use the following variables:
-
RT (with the response time in milliseconds, that is the time of the key down event)
-
TT (the time the key has been released)
-
STATUS (with values CORRECT,WRONG, or TIMEOUT, or 1,2, or 3)
readkeys
readkeys is completely different from readkey.
Here is an overview of the differences and similarities:
readkey |
readkeys |
Just one key press and RT |
One or more key presses and reaction times |
Useful for most experiments |
Useful for psycholinguistic and memory experiments |
Does not automatically show the key |
Automatically shows the typed in keys (but there is option to hide them) |
not suitable for multiline text input |
Example use of readkeys
-
You want that people enter a word or a number
-
You want to know for each keypress when exactly it was typed in
-
You want to check if people corrected mistakes with backspace
For entering longer texts, however, we recommend the textbox. |
The readkeys command has many options, so it may look a bit more difficult (see list below). |
The command allows for delete character. Return key "ends" the readkeys input. |
You can have letters/numbers evenly spaced or just normal. You can even ask people to type letters without showing them. |
bitmaps
apple ## a picture of an apple
empty_box ## an empty rectangle used as letter placeholder
fonts
arial 20
task my_task
text color yellow
show bitmap apple
readkeys "apple" 10000
save RT STATUS
A more complex function using the readkeys option statements.
Note, by default:
-
the typed letters will be shown on screen (you can hide them with readkeys option hide)
-
there are no placeholders shown (see example below for how to show placeholders)
-
the font color is white by default (you can set it differently with, for example textcolor yellow)
-
the size of the maximum array is 100 (which is in practical terms way longer than what you could need)
-
the letters will be shown starting at the center of the screen
-
the first defined font (in fonts section) will be used
-
the default spacing of letters will be 25 pixels
Readkeys options:
-
readkeys option size number : how many letters can be typed maximally.
-
readkeys option show : the typed letters will be shown on screen (starting at screen center)
-
readkeys option show x_position y_position : the typed letters will be shown as indicated by the x/y position
-
readkeys option hide : the typed letters will be not be shown on screen
-
readkeys option space number : The letters will be spaced by this number of pixels
-
readkeys option placeholders image : There will be placeholders in the shape of the indicated image (as defined in bitmaps)
-
readkeys option placeholders Width Height : The placeholders will be rectangles of size Width by Height
bitmaps
apple ## a picture of an apple
empty_box ## an empty rectangle used as letter placeholder
fonts
my_arial arial 20
task my_task
text color yellow
show bitmap apple
text color red
readkeys option size 5
readkeys option show 100 50
readkeys option placeholders empty_box
readkeys font font1
readkeys "apple" 10000
save RT STATUS WORD
Some explanations about the above example:
-
text color red: The readkeys command will show letters in red
-
readkeys option size 5: Maximally 5 letters can be typed
-
readkeys option placeholders empty_box: The placehold will be the bitmap image empty_box as given by the user in the bitmaps list
-
readkeys option show 100 50: The typed letters will be shown on screen at position 100 50.
-
readkeys option font my_arial: The font my_arial will be used
In the example above, the final readkeys line waits for 10000 ms until people have entered one or more letters (participant pressing the return/enter key ends the entry). If people entered "apple", they status is set to "correct" (i.e., value 1).
textbox
The textbox creates a box in which people can enter text. Unlike the readkeys statement, the textbox allows special character entry as well (including Chinese text entry, special characters in European languages, and so on.
You can also use textbox to simply show a long wrapped text.
The textbox was introduced in the 3.4.* series.0 and will be developed further. Currently, a textbox is shown with the instruction textbox new and you can provide multiple arguments. Below is an example in which the following are used: x-position, y-position, width, height, textcolor, background color, textfont size and font family (such as arial or times).
The focus is used for when people need to enter text.
In the following example, the participant needs to enter words followed by enter. The textbox instruction draws a 700 by 500 pixels with a blue background and with white text on it. The text that will appear in it will have size 24 in font Arial.
The textbox is followed by a readkey. If you set the time to 999999 it basically waits for 999 seconds, which is quite long for an experiment.
task test
keys enter
show text "Enter words followed by Enter" 0 -280
textbox new 0 0 700 500 white blue 24 arial
textbox 1 focus
readkey 1 999999
set %mytext textbox
textbox 1 clear
save %mytext
block x
tasklist
test 1
end
The full syntax of the relevant functions is as follows
textbox new # just a basic one 90% of screen textbox new [x][y][w][h] textbox new [x][y][w][h] [col][bg] textbox new [x][y][w][h] [col][bg] [font][fontsize] textbox [n] clear textbox [n] erase textbox [n] focus textbox [n] lock set %my_var textbox
See also the lesson about textbox to get a better explanation. here. |
readmouse
For the non-click version of readmouse, this has changed slightly in version 3.1.0 |
Similar to readkey, you can check if the participant clicked the mousekey, and if so, if the mouse was where it should be. You can also just wait for the mouse to be moved into a certain area of the screen. In fact, you can check if the location of a specific stimulus has been touched with the mouse (bitmap, or rectangle, or text).
Here are the different options
-
Wait until the mouse is clicked and the mouse is in the correct location
-
Wait until the participant moves the mouse to the correct location
In the following example, we show two rectangles, and we want that the participant mouse the mouse over the green one (the first bitmap), which is positioned on the left (-200). We give maximally 5 seconds, that is 5000 milliseconds.
task checkMouse
show rectangle -200 0 40 40 0 255 0 # green rect, left
show rectangle 200 0 40 40 255 0 0 # red rect, right
readmouse 1 5000
save STATUS RT
In the example above, you get a correct STATUS if the left rectangle was moved over (hovered over), and you get STATUS wrong if the right rectangle is moved over. |
In case there are more stimuli on the screen, it is recommended you use the range parameter, as shown below. |
task checkMouse
show rectangle 0 0 700 500 100 100 100
show rectangle -200 0 40 40 0 255 0 # green rect, left
show rectangle 200 0 40 40 255 0 0 # red rect, right
readmouse 2 5000 range 2 3
save STATUS RT
In the example above, the first rectangle is a grey rectangle that is just a background. Because that rectangle is almost as big as the whole experiment screen, we need to tell PsyToolkit that we do not care about that stimulus in regard to the mouse being over it. To do so, we use range in the readmouse instruction. This means, in this case, only consider if stimulus 2 or stimulus 3 has been hovered over.
In the following example, we have exactly the same task, but we want that the mouse is being clicked as well as in the first stimulus. We add the "l" argument, with "l" standing for the left mousebutton. In Javascript, it is recommended only to use the left mousebutton, because the right mouse button might show a browser-specific menu, which is obviously not something you want.
task checkMouse
show rectangle -200 0 40 40 0 255 0 # green rect, left
show rectangle 200 0 40 40 255 0 0 # red rect, right
readmouse l 1 5000
save STATUS RT
There is one issue that arises if you have stimuli that overlap each other. Imagine the following situation. You have one big rectangle that is just their to show a clear yellow rectangle, and on top of that is a much smaller rectangle. If you want that people click that second smaller rectangle, you need to tell the computer that you do not care about the first one. The way to do that is to specify the range of bitmaps that you are interested in. See example below:
task checkMouse
show rectangle -200 0 400 400 255 255 0 # big yellow rectangle
show rectangle 50 0 40 40 255 0 0 # small red one
readmouse l 2 5000 range 2 2 # wait for second one being clicked
save STATUS RT
Finally, there is a special hovering case, where you can ask readmouse only to respond if the mouse hovers over the requested target, and not to respond with error for any other stimuli. For this, you just add the option find.
task checkMouse
show rectangle -200 0 40 40 0 255 0 # green rect, left
show rectangle 200 0 40 40 255 0 0 # red rect, right
readmouse 1 5000 find
save STATUS RT
In the above case, you can hover over the right rectangle, but PsyToolkit will fully ignore this, and only look for the left stimulus. This can be useful in certain situations. For example, if you want that a participant finds one object among many. The participant might hover over all the objects until the person has seen the target object. You could, of course, also do that with a "range", but this is shorted and for this, you do not need to know exactly the numbers for the range parameter, so it makes things simpler.
drag
The drag function allows you to drag a stimulus from one location to another. There are many options to do so. Often it is easiest to look at a simple example first. Best to read the lesson about https:/lessons/drag.html[drag] for detailed examples.
Below are also basic examples, but first an overview. Note that the italic values need to be replaced with numbers of variables. What is in square brackets are various different options. More details further down.
In its simplest possible form, you can have an example like this. Note that you will always need to provide *drag option" lines to tell PsyToolkit which stimulus needs to be dragged.
In this example, you show one stimulus (stimulus number 1) in yellow (yellow is RGB: 255 255 0). You tell PsyToolkit that the stimulus that can be dragged from somewhere is in this cases stimulus number 1. You give up to 60000 milliseconds (that is 60 seconds) for the dragging.
task my_drag
show rectangle 0 0 100 100 255 255 0
drag option from 1
drag 99999
block test
tasklist
my_drag 1
end
There are more options. For example, you can PsyToolkit that there are more stimuli that can be dragged from. For *drag option from" you can specify multiple stimuli or even an array, as in the example below.
task my_drag
show rectangle -100 0 100 100 255 0 0 # red
show rectangle 0 0 100 100 0 255 0 # green
show rectangle 100 0 100 100 255 255 255 # white
drag option from 1 2
drag 99999
block test
tasklist
my_drag 1
end
The drag option to specifies the value(s) of one or more stimuli. In its simplest form, you can have an example such as this:
task my_drag
show rectangle 100 0 120 120 0 255 0 # green
show rectangle -100 0 100 100 255 0 0 # red
drag option from 2
drag option to 1
drag 99999
show text "Now wait 1000 ms" 0 150
delay 1000
block test
tasklist
my_drag 1
end
To the above exaple, you could have added drag option snap to let the red rectangle snap exactly on the green one. You can even specify whether how you want to snap, here are some examples: |
dragging
Dragging allows your participant to continue dragging stimuli around the screen until done. When they are done (but before maximum response time) they need to press the exit button. For this, you can specify a specific stimulus.
All the options of drag can also be used for dragging, but there is one additional option.
In the example below, two red and two green rectangles are shown. A fifth yellow rectangle (with the text "Exit" on it) functions as an exit button. You need to drag the red rectangles onto the green ones.
Note that if you do not drag them on the targets, they jump back to their original positions. |
task my_dragging
show rectangle 100 -100 120 120 0 255 0 # green
show rectangle 100 100 120 120 0 255 0 # green
show rectangle -100 -100 100 100 255 0 0 # red
show rectangle -100 100 100 100 255 0 0 # red
show rectangle 250 0 100 80 255 255 0
show text "Exit" 250 0 0 0 0
drag option from 3 4
drag option to 1 2
drag option snap center center
drag option exit 5
dragging 60000
save RT
block test
tasklist
my_dragging 1
end
Variables for use with drag and dragging
The following variables can be used for accessing information about drag actions. You do not necessarily need these.
DRAGGED contains the number of the stimulus a participant dragged. This is interesting when a participant can choose to drag one out of multiple stimuli.
DRAGGED_ON contains the number of the stimulus a participant dragged onto.
DRAGGED_CHANGED Contains 1 if the participant dragged a stimulus, and 0 if nothing was dragged.
DRAG_OLD_X If a stimulus was dragged, its previous X position before dragging started.
DRAG_OLD_Y If a stimulus was dragged, its previous Y position before
DRAG_X If a stimulus was dragged, its current X position.
DRAG_Y If a stimulus was dragged, its current Y position.
Experiments using drag or dragging can be quite complex. Learn how to write these in the lessons section. |
progress
PsyToolkit can show a progress bar. This can be useful if you want to tell people how many trials they have done and how long it will take to finish.
Progress bars can distract. If you do an experiment studying visual attention or memory, you need to think about whether you think looking at the progress bar may interfere with the cognitive process you are studying. |
It is actually really easy to show. There are a number of options to color and shape the progress bar the way you like, but the default is a simple narrow horizontal progress bar at the top of the screen.
The command is simple: show progressbar done todo
Whereby the first number is the trials done so far and the second number is the total trials the participant will have to do.
In the example below, the show progress command shows the progress bar. It is followed by the TRIALCOUNT (trials done so far) and the total number of trials (here for example 30).
task exampletask
show progress TRIALCOUNT 30
delay 1000
show mystimulus
There is a special set of options that you can set within your task (and not in the options section). Below is an example in which you can set the two colors (one for the color of the trials done and one for the background color, by default these are green and grey, respectively).
task exampletask
progress option size 0 290 600 10
progress option color1 255 255 0
progress option color2 128 128 128
progress option between 5
show progress TRIALCOUNT 30
delay 1000
show mystimulus
These options have the following meaning.
-
size: X position (middle of progressbar), Y position, width, height
-
color1: The color for the trials done (in Red/Green/Blue format)
-
color2: The color for the trials done (in Red/Green/Blue format)
-
between: The space between trials (for example, choose 2 for small gap between each progress point)
Note, instead of color1 and color2 you can also just write:
progress option colors 255 255 0 128 128 128
choose
Under certain circumstances, you want to ask the participant to click multiple stimuli on the screen. Ideally, you would want to allow the participant to select objects and deselect as well. This is all possible with the function choose.
In short, choose will let the participant click on a range stimuli, show a symbol on top of the stimuli, and when clicked again the symbol will disappear.
It is easiest to learn how this works from an example.
In the following example, there are three symbols shown on the screen that can be selected with a selector bitmap. Options for this command can be set before choose is called with choose option. The participant has 60 seconds (60,000 milliseconds).
bitmaps
markingsymbol
house
ball
car
exitsymbol1 ## shown when at least 2 selected
exitsymbol2 ## shown when not enough are selected
task clickMysymbols
show bitmap house # bitmap 1
show bitmap ball # bitmap 2
show bitmap car # bitmap 3
choose option select markingsymbol
choose option minselect 2
choose option exit exitsymbol1 exitsymbol2 350 250
choose 60000 1 3
save RT CHOSEN_N CHOSEN_1 CHOSEN_2 CHOSEN_3
Note the following things about the above example:
-
There are three images on the screen.
-
If you click an image, the "markingsymbol" image will be put on top of it
-
If you click it again, this "markingsymbol" will be removed
-
The RT is the response time until the exitsymbol was clicked
-
CHOSEN_N contains number of selected objects
-
CHOSEN_1, CHOSEN_2, CHOSEN_3, etc contains the number of the object. They will always be in numerical order.
There are various other options:
-
choose option minselect: The minimum to be selected objects
-
choose option maxselect: The maxumum to be selected objects
-
choose option sprites: Select from sprites instead of static stimuli
-
choose option keep: Keeps the participant selected selector images on screen (the default is that they are erased immedeately when the choose function is over)
The choose command is particularly useful for memory tasks. |
rate
Sometimes people want a Likert scale in their experiment. For example, you might show a shopping item and ask people, how likely would you be buying this in the next week on a scale from one to five. This is now super easy to do.
bitmaps
something_for_sale
task wantit
show bitmap something_for_sale -200 0
rate 10000 5
save RATE RATE_RT RATE_STATUS
This gives you the most basic type of a Likert scale. By default, the points of the likert scale are yellow circles and left and right, you see a small white box. In reality, you want a lot more. For that, you can give options. First of all, you can select a location with the "option pos" followed by an x and y coordinate.
In the example below, the scale is now shown left of center.
bitmaps
something_for_sale
task wantit
show bitmap something_for_sale -200 0
rate option pos -200 0
rate 10000 5
save RATE RATE_RT RATE_STATUS
In the example below, we add nicer bitmaps instead of the default drawing symbols. In PsyToolkit, there is the option labels for rate, so that you can make a bitmap with the label texts "not at all" and "very much" or just simple arrows in the example below. The "option items" just specifies the bitmap of the clickable points on the scale.
Also, in the example below we have 7 items and people must make choice within 5 seconds.
bitmaps
something_for_sale
left_arrow
right_arrow
my_rectangle
task wantit
show bitmap something_for_sale -200 0
rate option pos -200 0
rate option labels left_arrow right_arrow
rate option items my_rectangle
rate 5000 7
save RATE RATE_RT RATE_STATUS
save
Saves variables. Typically, a task ends with a save line.
The save instruction is of critical importance, because it makes sure that the information you need for your data analysis is being stored. By default, PsyToolkit does not keep any information (unlike some other experiment software). The user needs to tell PsyToolkit exactly which information is being stored to the data file. |
Typically, you will want to save what the current condition is, the current block (if you have more than one block), and at least the response time (RT), and whether the participant responded correctly or not (STATUS). The examples on this website can help you to understand this. |
The best place for the save command is at the end of your task description. |
save [list of variables] |
save BLOCKNAME RT STATUS
sound
Used to play sounds. A sound just starts playing the code continues. If you want to do nothing during the sound, you need to have it followed by a delay. You can also stop the sound at any time using the silence instruction.
sound [sound name (as defined in 'sounds'] |
sound MySoundFile
delay 200
silence MySoundFile
if
You can use if to only carry out some commands. This is often needed for showing a feedback message if people make a mistake. The opposite of if is fi.
task MyTask
show bitmap stimulus
readkey 1 1000
if STATUS == CORRECT
show bitmap WellDone
delay 1000
clear -1
fi
if STATUS != CORRECT
show bitmap Mistake
delay 2000
clear -1
fi
if $x == &y
show bitmap SmileyFace $x $y
fi
task MyTask
show bitmap stimulus
readkey 1 1000
if STATUS == CORRECT
show bitmap WellDone
delay 1000
clear -1
else
show bitmap Mistake
delay 2000
clear -1
fi
if $x == &y
show bitmap SmileyFace $x $y
fi
The statement else need to be on its own line |
With if, you can also compare two arrays
task MyTask
set &a 1 2 3 4
set &b 1 2 3 4
if &&a == &&b
show text "Arrays a and b are exactly the same"
else
show text "Arrays a and b are different"
fi
while
Similar to if, you can put code in a "while loop". The while loop is ended with the while-end statement.
Everything between a while and while-end statement will be carried out until the condition on the while line is satisfied.
task MyTask
set $mycounter 0
while $mycounter < 10
set $x random -200 200
set $y random -200 200
show bitmap SmileyFace $x $y
set $mycounter increase
while-end
If you make a mistake, the computer might be stuck in the while loop. For example, if you do not increase the value of $mycounter in the above example, the code will be stuck and keep drawing new SmileyFace symbols at random positions. |
less often used instructions
Some functions are rarely used or necessary, yet they add to the potential of PsyToolkit scripting.
font
You can set the font of the next *show text" instruction.
fonts
MySmallFont arial.ttf 20
MyBigFont arial.ttf 50
task Mytask
font MySmallFont
show text "small" 0 0
font MyBigFont
show text "small" 0 100
very advanced instructions
end
You can end the task at any point in your list of statements in a task. You can even end the current tasklist or the whole experiment. All you need to do is end task , end tasklist , or end experiment
This can be useful, for example, when people are training and you want to stop the block of trials when they made a mistake.
This is, however, a statement that you will rarely need.
timestamp
timestamp can be used to capture the current time. This can be handy if you are checking the time past in a while..while-end loop. It can also be handy if you are trying to debug code and check if the timing is as expected.
You can get the timestamp-diff between two timestamp variables using the set command as in the example below. Timestamps cannot be directly accessed or used other than through set timestamp-diff
task MyTask
timestamp MyFirstTimestamp
delay 1000
timestamp MySecondTimestamp
set $x timestamp-diff MyFirstTimestamp MySecondTimestamp
save $x
tables
Tables have lines and rows. Each time a table is being used in a task, one of its rows is chosen. Each column can be referred to using the @ sign. For example, @2 refers to the second column on the row that is being chosen for a given task trial. In the block, the user can specify how table lines are selected. The default is randomly, but there are alternative ways (for example: fixed order, repeat on trial).
table MyTable
10 2 bitmap1
-10 1 bitmap2
table MyTable
10 2 bitmap1 "condition one"
-10 1 bitmap2 "condition two"
In a task that uses a table (as instructed with the table instruction), each column can be referred to with the @ sign. Thus @2 refers to the second column of the table row that has been selected.
Each time that a task is being carried out, only one table-row of the associated table is being selected. By default, a row is chosen randomly. There are other ways to choose table rows (using the tasklist instruction in blocks). |
In each task trial with a table, you have access not only access to the columns, but you can also get the number of the tablerow. The variable is called TABLEROW. This can be handy for saving data. If there is a lot of important data in each tablerow that you want to use in your analysis, you want to make sure which TABLEROW was being used.
blocks
In experiments, trials come in blocks. A block of trials means that the participant does, for example, 100 trials of the Stroop task. Instead, you might want to have a break in the middle, so you could create instead 2 blocks of 50 trials. Blocks call tasks, so you really must have at least one block in your PsyToolkit script. Blocks can be complex, but they can also be very easy, like this example below.
Blocks are the best place to add your instructions for participants. Check this lesson about this out. |
block MyBlock
tasklist
strooptask 100
end
showing bitmaps, playing sounds, and waiting for keys
Often, you want to show instructions to participants. You can show bitmaps, and you can show series of bitmaps participants can scroll forward and backwards through (pagers).
bitmap
The bitmap is simple. You often want to show a bitmap with an instruction and then wait for a keypress.
block Myblock
bitmap MyInstruction
wait_for_key
tasklist
strooptask 100
end
Showing a bitmap in a task and in a block is different. Here in blocks, do not put the word show before the bitmap. In tasks you have to. There are good reasons for this distinction (in tasks, stimuli are fast paced, whereas in blocks, they are thought of as instructions). |
sound
As in the task, you can start a sound with sound and stop it with silence
block Myblock
sound welcometune
bitmap MyInstruction
wait_for_key
silence welcometune
tasklist
strooptask 100
end
delay
As in tasks, you can put in a delay. Sometimes this is nice for countdowns.
block Myblock
bitmap number3
delay 500
bitmap number2
delay 500
bitmap number1
delay 500
tasklist
mytask 100
end
message
Instead of showing a bitmap and waiting for a key, you can combine this in one command, called message.
block Myblock
message MyInstruction
tasklist
strooptask 100
end
message ThankYouBitmap
block Myblock
message MyInstruction b
tasklist
strooptask 100
end
message ThankYouBitmap
You can use the mouse key instead of the keyboard. Make sure you instruct this clearly on the message. The participant will then continue when clicking (or touching) anywhere on the instruction bitmap. |
block Myblock
message MyInstruction mouse
tasklist
strooptask 100
end
message ThankYouBitmap
pager
Sometimes you want a series of instructions and let the user browse through it. In other words, you have series of pictures (or "pages", hence the word "pager") that you want to show one after another.
The pager command let you do exactly this.
Users can use the arrow keys and use the Q key to get out of it. The space bar will go to the next image/page. Make sure that all these bitmaps are in the bitmap section.
Alternatively, you can use the mouse instead of the keyboard (below). |
Make sure you tell in the instructions how users browse through it and how they get out of it. |
block Myblock
pager MyInstruction1 MyInstruction2 MyInstruction3 MyInstruction4
tasklist
strooptask 100
end
Alternatively, you can use the mouse (or touch) to scroll through the instruction pages. For this, you need to give some more information. If you are a beginner, try the basic pagers as explained above first.
The mouse/touch pager is sometimes more useful (e.g., touch screen experiments). |
In the example below, we use the pager option to tell the computer the following:
-
Use the mouse for the pager
-
The bitmap named "back" should be presented at position -200,200
-
The bitmap named "next" should be presented at position 0,200
-
The bitmap named "start" should be presented at position 200,200
Thus, apart from the instructions, on each screen there are 3 bitmaps, one for going back through the instructions, one for going forward, and one for leaving the instructions. The latter one is only shown once the participant has gone through them all.
Note, for this experiment, you need to have bitmaps for the images: back, next, start, MyInstruction1, MyInstruction2, MyInstruction3.
block Myblock
pager option mouse back -200 200 next 0 200 start 200 200
pager MyInstruction1 MyInstruction2 MyInstruction3
tasklist
strooptask 100
end
setting variables
Sometimes you want to set a global variable. You can do that just like you would do in tasks (including arrays). For example, you might set the maximum response time to a higher value during training.
block MyTraining
pager MyInstruction1 MyInstruction2 MyInstruction3 MyInstruction4
set &MaxResponseTime 2000
tasklist
strooptask 20
end
block MyRealDatablock
set &MaxResponseTime 1000
pager MyInstruction1 MyInstruction2 MyInstruction3 MyInstruction4
tasklist
strooptask 100
end
tasklist
Each block has a task list. This describes which tasks are being called, how many times, and in what order. You might have one or more tasks. It is rare to have multiple tasks, but in taskswitching paradigms this might be the case.
On each line of the task list, you need at the very least provide the name of the task and the number of trials.
block MyTraining
tasklist
strooptask 20
end
You can specify that you want that whenever people make a mistake, the same trial should be repeated, using repeat_on_error. If you use this, you need to specify what is considered to be an error using the error statement (see example below).
Repeat_on_error only works when you use tables, random variables are not set to the same values. |
table MyTable
"condition1" 255 0 0 1 ## respond with "r" to red rectangle
"condition2" 0 255 0 2 ## respond with "g" to green rectangle
task Mytask
table MyTable
keys r g
show rectangle 0 0 50 50 @2 @3 @4
readkey @5 2000
if STATUS == WRONG
error ## this makes this trial an error for the repeat_on_error process
fi
save @1 STATUS RT
block MyTraining
tasklist
Mytask 10 repeat_on_error
end
In the example above, you see a red or green rectangle (presented at position 0,0, which is screen center, and 50px wide and 50px high). You need to respond with the "r" or "g" key. You might wonder why you would need to explicitly specify what an error is if you use "repeat_on_error". This is a good question, because in the above example it is indeed an error if the person responds with the wrong key. But there are experiments in which you actually want that people do not respond (that is STATUS is TIMEOUT). And in some experiments, there is more than one response per trial. The "error" statement gives you full control over what an error is and what not. |
In some experiments, you might want that all participants carry out the trials in exactly the same order. You can just run through each line of your task table. You use the fixed option for this.
block MyTraining
tasklist
strooptask 20 fixed
end
In some experiments, you might want that participants do at least a certain number of trials correct. You use the correct option for this. In the example below, the participant needs to have done 10 trials correct, but after 100 trials it will stop no matter how many the participant did correct. In the allcorrect variant, they must be consecutively correct.
block MyTraining
tasklist
strooptask 10 correct 100
end
block MyTraining
tasklist
strooptask 10 allcorrect 100
end
Finally, you sometimes want that trials do never repeat. There are three ways to get this in the tasklist statement
-
all_before_repeat : do all trials as in the table, randomly selected, but run them all before chosen again
-
no_repeat : never immediately repeat a condition from the table (i.e., the same row of a table will not be repeated in next trial)
-
fixed : run trials in the order as they are in the table
In the following example, there are only three entries on the following list. They will not be repeated until they are all done. You can explicitly add that you never want any repeat.
table MyTable
bitmapBlue
bitmapRed
bitmapGreen
task strooptask
table MyTable
keys space
show bitmap @1
readkey 1
block MyTraining
tasklist
strooptask 10 all_before_repeat no_repeat
end
If you have fixed in consecutive blocks, but use the same task and table, you will continue at the table row where you had left. This is a handy feature in case you have many trials you want to go through, but sometimes people need a break. |
You can use the tasklist line option equal to ensure equal numbers of each table row are done. Ensure that the number of trials is a multiple of the table rows. Thus, if you have 4 table rows (that is, "conditions" in your experiment), you will have to have at least 4 trials, or 8 or 12 or any other multiple of 4.
block my_block
tasklist
my_task1 16 equal
end
Make sure that you always have an end at the end of your tasklist |
maxtime
You can set the maximum duration of a block (in milliseconds, seconds, or minutes). It is very simple. Here is an example of a block that can last for maximally 2 minutes.
You can still have instructions before the task list (e.g., as a message or pager, they are not counted toward the maxtime). |
block MyTraining
maxtime 2m
tasklist
strooptask 10 all_before_repeat no_repeat
end
block MyTraining
maxtime 100s
tasklist
strooptask 10 all_before_repeat no_repeat
end
block MyTraining
maxtime &MyVariable
tasklist
strooptask 10 all_before_repeat no_repeat
end
feedback
Feedback can be used to create feedback to the participant, for example about the average response speed. Creating feedback is part of the "block" structure. Feedback is described in a separate document, because it is of advanced use: how to use feedback
blockorder
Advanced use |
By default, blocks are carried out in the order they are described in your code. You can alter this order by rearranging the block code, but there is an easier and quicker way: blockorder.
Using the blockorder statement, you can list the blocks you want to do and the order you want them in.
The idea behind this function is just speed of coding, and ease of changing the order (for example for counterbalancing).
In the following example, imagine there is code for three blocks, called training, test1, and test2. The following example show how to set the order of these blocks in different ways.
blockorder
training
test1
test2
Most importantly, you can have more than one blockorder. When you have more than one blockorder, PsyToolkit will choose one at random. Which blockorder will be chosen will be stored in the variable BLOCKORDER. By default, this variable contains value 1, but if you have given, say, 3 blockorders, this value can be between values 1 and 3. Below is an example of this use.
In the example below, the computer will choose one of the two blockorders at random. That is what the computer will do when it see two blockorders. This is great for counterbalancing, for example if you want that some participants start with one task and others with the another task. |
task color_task
keys a l
readkey 1 1000
readkey
save BLOCKORDER BLOCKNAME TASKNAME RT
task shape_task
keys a l
readkey 1 1000
readkey
save BLOCKORDER BLOCKNAME TASKNAME RT
blockorder
training
color_task
shape_task
blockorder
training
shape_task
color_task
include (advanced use only)
You can include another file into your script. This can be useful if you work with very large tables which you want to store in a separate file. Any line starting with include followed by a filename will use that filename at that place in the script.
Included files must be in the same directory as the main script file. |
table My_Large_Table
include tablefile.txt
task some_task
table My_Large_Table
show bitmap @1
delay 100
block test
tasklist
some_task 10
end
Included files (like tablefile.txt in the example) should not include other files using include. |
part (for advanced user)
You can write "snippets" of a few lines and include them elsewhere in your script. This is handy if you have multiple tasks or blocks which are broadly the same except for a few lines. This way can use a part instead of re-typing the whole script.
part just replaces the text into your code. The main aim is to have shorter more efficient code without repetitions of the same stuff. |
part showAnimatedSquare
show rectangle 0 0 50 50 255 255 0
delay 100
show rectangle 0 0 100 100 255 255 0
delay 100
show rectangle 0 0 150 150 255 255 0
delay 100
part removeSquare
clear -1 -2 -3
task some_task
keys space
part showAnimatedSquare
readkey 1 1000
part removeSquare
delay 100
task another_task
keys a
part showAnimatedSquare
readkey 1 5000
part removeSquare
delay 200
block test
tasklist
some_task 10
end
block test
tasklist
another_task 10
end
Overview of all the system variables
Below a quick overview of almost all the "system" variables that you can use in your experiment script. They are described in detail in the text, but here they are all together. They typically all contain a numerical value, except for TASKNAME, and BLOCKNAME. The latter two are only of use in the save instruction. The choose-related variables are not listed. They all need to be written in capitals. |
RT |
last response time, from readkey or readmouse |
STATUS |
from readkey(s)/readmouse, can be CORRECT, WRONG, TIMEOUT (or 1, 2, 3) |
KEY |
the key that has been pressed in last readkey/keystatus (a number) |
UNDER_MOUSE |
the number of the stimulus the participant clicked on |
MOUSE_X |
the X position of the mouse in last readmouse statement |
MOUSE_Y |
the Y position of the mouse in last readmouse statement |
TABLEROW |
randomly chosen once per trial |
TASKNAME |
the name of the task the trial is in |
TRIALCOUNT |
the number of trials so far in any task |
BLOCKNUMBER |
the number of the current block. Starts with 1. |
BLOCKNAME |
the name of the current block the trial is in |
BLOCKORDER |
Is normally 1, but if multiple blockorders defined, gives the number of the randomly chosen blockorder |
RATE |
The chosen value of rating scale |
RATE_RT |
The RT of the rating process |
RATE_STATUS |
will be 1 if rated, or 3 if there is a timeout (there is no correct response, of course) |
SHOW_COUNTER |
task my_task
show rectangle 0 0 100 100 0 255 0 # green rectangle
show rectangle -200 0 100 100 255 0 0 # red rectangle
show rectangle 200 0 100 100 0 0 255 # blue rectangle
show text SHOW_COUNTER ## this will be 3
delay 1000
clear SHOW_COUNTER ## delete last shown stimulus
SHOW_COUNTER repaces the show-counter set option. It is rarely needed, but in advanced code with many stimuli, it can help to later clear stimuli from the screen (because you might loose track of stimulus counter numbers). |
Links to all instructions
(this list is not yet complete)
Options: origin| bitmapdir| sounddir| fontdir| fullscreen| resolution| version| mouse| variable| window| screensize| screendistance| vsync| egi|escape| parallelport| pcidio24|cedrus| iolab| eye-tracker| executable
Medium advanced instructions: readmouse| if| while| move| relocate| hide| unhide readmouse| drag dragging tablerow
Special instructions: include| part| blockorder
Overview of system variables